Pre-emptive

Eventually the need to make the slides and actually deliver the vision lecture outpaced the urge to blog prospective content, and today was crunch day. I think it went okay, although I wound up skipping the attention section due to lack of time. Two hours is both a fucking eternity and not quite enough. Students were spared the cocktail party analogy with rock stars and crypto bros doing coke in the toilets and nobody shutting the hell up. Maybe it’s for the best.

I will write and post the remaining episodes — I have already performed the material, after all — but I need to catch a breath first. Similarly, pre-rec videos will be found on this YouTube playlist, although “pre-” is a bit of a misnomer since only the first two are up so far; again, they’re coming, but there are a lot of competing claims on my time just now. Most urgently, content for tomorrow’s lab session and the late summer exam for COMP0142.

In the meantime, have some slides. (At some point I will tweak these too, at least to comply with license attribution requirements, but for now I doubt anyone will notice.)

6. Side By Side

Signals from the retina — finally — travel out from the eye along the optic nerve — through the blind spot — and head to the brain for really quite a lot more processing. Much of which, if we’re honest, is not very well understood at all.

The first major stop along the visual pathway is the optic chiasm, a sort of traffic intersection where the signals from both eyes come together, only to be split up and shipped out in different company. The signals from the nasal half of each retina — ie, the side closest to the nose — cross over to the other side of the brain, where they join the signals from the temporal half of the other retina — the side closest to the temple. Image formation in the eye, like any camera, flips up-down and left-right. So the nasal retina of the left eye captures the left hand side of the field of view, as does the temporal retina of the right eye, while the right nasal and left temporal retinas capture the right hand side of the field of view.

From this point on, the visual information is grouped according to which side of the world it’s coming from, rather than which eye. Everything over on the right hand side of the world gets routed to the left hemisphere of the brain, while everything on the left hand side of the world gets routed to the right hemisphere. Each hemisphere still keeps track of which eye each of the signals for its half of the world originated from, but in a real sense it just loses sight of the whole other half of the world. (It’s actually a bit less than half — there’s some shared space in the middle that gets covered by both sides of the brain.)

The hemifields from each eye overlap quite a bit, but between them they cover more territory than either individually. Only the nasal edition has a blind spot, for example — one of the reasons we are almost never aware of this hole in our vision is that it can be filled in from the other eye. Though the more fundamental reason is that our brain doesn’t want us to be aware, and it is 100% calling the shots.

Because the eyes are spatially separated, they see things from a different angle. (Your life is a sham, etc. If I were properly committed to the pre-rec version of this lecture, which rest assured I am not, I would probably have to drag up at this point for a stupid 2 second Zaza insert. Y’all ain’t ready. Fortunately, neither am I.) Comparing and contrasting the two views is super useful for stereoscopic depth perception, so it makes sense to bring them both together for visual processing. The half view split is really quite unintuitive, though, and it can lead to some apparently strange pathologies.

If you lose — or for whatever reason start without — sight in one of your eyes, your field of view will be a bit more limited, as will your depth perception, but both sides of your brain will still be processing both halves of the world and you’ll perceive both left and right. But if the visual processing pathway on one side of your brain is disrupted, downstream of the optic chiasm — say by a stroke or traumatic brain injury — then your ability to see that half of the world may be impaired as well. This is hemianopsia — sometimes hemianopia — or half blindness. In this condition your eyes still work just fine, capturing images like everyone else’s. You’re still sensing both halves of the world, but you can’t perceive one of them.

After the optic chiasm, the visual signals travel to the the thalamus, which is kind of the main routing hub for sensory information coming into the brain; specifically to the LGN or lateral geniculate nucleus — nuclei really, since there’s one each side. Exactly what perceptual processing goes on there is not known for sure, but importantly the LGN doesn’t just take feedforward sensory input from the eyes, it also takes feedback input from further along in the visual system — indeed, there are more feedback connections than feedforward.

One of the things the LGN is believed to do is to take a kind of temporal derivative of the visual information — comparing it with what was previously seen and picking out changes, in a sort of analogue of the spatial differencing performed in the retinal ganglion cells. Again, we’re fine tuning for salience and novelty is interesting.

Another of the things the LGN is thought to be doing is applying some attentional filtering — selecting some aspects of the visual signals and deselecting others to focus in on what we’re especially interested in perceiving right now — what we’re paying attention to. Unlike the relatively static processes of change detection, this kind of selectivity is dynamic — it’s not always picking out the same features, it varies from moment to moment, depending on what we’re doing and thinking at the time. Some of those feedback signals from the cortex are saying, in effect: “boring, boring, meh, boring, whoa hang on a second there buddy, tell me more”. And the LGN does.

All of this is still happening at a pretty low level — tweaking nerve firings corresponding to tiny patches of space and moments of time, not yet assembled into objects or events, not yet translated into knowledge or behaviour. But these fragments are the matter of visual perception, and yet again we see that the brain is right there in the mix at every stage, exerting itself, sifting and organising, amplifying and attenuating, making connections and also, whenever necessary, making stuff up.